2011 to 2022 Autism "surge"
ASPartOfMe
Veteran
Joined: 25 Aug 2013
Age: 67
Gender: Male
Posts: 35,980
Location: Long Island, New York
Autism Diagnoses Surge Among Kids, Adults
In a review of electronic health records and insurance claims data for about 12 million people between 2011 and 2022, researchers found the odds of an autism diagnosis “increased substantially.”
The study published this week in the journal JAMA Network Open indicates that autism prevalence jumped from 2.3 per 1,000 to 6.3 per 1,000 during the period studied.
The biggest change was seen in adults ages 26 to 34, a group that saw a 450% increase in autism diagnoses, the study found. There was also significant growth in diagnoses of female versus male children.
The highest prevalence of autism was seen among children ages 5 to 8, the study found. Kids from racial and ethnic minority groups saw bigger increases than white children, though that trend did not hold in adults.
For the study, researchers at Kaiser Permanente analyzed records for patients at eight health care systems that are part of a consortium of research centers known as the Mental Health Research Network. Individuals were determined to be on the spectrum if they had an autism diagnosis under the ninth or tenth revisions of the International Classification of Diseases, a standardized system used to code medical conditions.
“The improvement and expansion of universal developmental screening likely accounts for some of the increase in diagnosis rates we found in this study,” said Luke Grosvenor, the study’s lead author and a research fellow at the Kaiser Permanente Division of Research. “The magnitude of the rate increases and variability by age, gender, race and ethnicity suggests factors other than improved screening are also contributing to the rate increases.”
The study stands out for looking at autism rates among people of all ages, as most prevalence research focuses on kids. However, the findings are largely consistent with data collected through the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, which tracks autism rates in children, the researchers said.
Autism Diagnosis Among US Children and Adults, 2011-2022
_________________
Professionally Identified and joined WP August 26, 2013
DSM 5: Autism Spectrum Disorder, DSM IV: Aspergers Moderate Severity
“My autism is not a superpower. It also isn’t some kind of god-forsaken, endless fountain of suffering inflicted on my family. It’s just part of who I am as a person”. - Sara Luterman
The key points from this from a statistical standpoint & view is:-
1. Diagnosis at 8 years old (anything else is irrelevant) as that is the fixed point the CDC measures each year. So if more 8 year olds are diagnosed then yes diagnosis is going up as that is the measuring point. If a 40 year old is diagnosed that statistically doesn't mean anything as 40 year olds stats are not measured properly.
2. Why does the CDC only measure at 8 years old? It should be 8 & 18, the reason 23.5% of 8 year olds are borderline ID, on top of the 37.9% already ID, so the correct % of ID in autism is not measured properly. At 18 researchers say this borderline group splits in two adding another 11% to ID making the true prevalence of ID in autism approx.49% at 18.
Approx half of autistic adults diagnosed at 8 years old will be ID as adults. This horror is generally off the radar and it explains why autistic adults have such a hidden hard time coping independently
https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/72/ss/ss7202a1.htm
2. Back to statistics if autism including ID, If that is going up then it is a very serious & negative thing
_________________
"The reasonable man adapts himself to the world; the unreasonable one persists in trying to adapt the world to himself. Therefore all progress depends upon the unreasonable man."
- George Bernie Shaw
lostonearth35
Veteran
Joined: 5 Jan 2010
Age: 50
Gender: Female
Posts: 12,688
Location: Lost on Earth, waddya think?
Gentleman Argentum
Veteran
Joined: 24 Aug 2019
Age: 54
Gender: Male
Posts: 850
Location: State of Euphoria
Back in the day - 1970s - only kids that got diagnosed, or paid any attention to by psychologists, were the ones really bad off. I would characterize psychological care back then as rudimentary at best. I don't know whether it has improved.
I think the higher diagnosis is probably the result of greater awareness, and the internet making research easy and convenient.
Thank you, Internet.
_________________
My magical motto is Animus facit nobilem. I like to read fantasy and weird fiction. Just a few of my favorite online things: music, chess, and dungeon crawl stone soup.
The subject of increased diagnosis was mentioned in a NCSA article a few days ago:-
https://www.ncsautism.org/blog//autism-explosion-2024
October 30, 2024
Though denialism remains in vogue (for now), there is overwhelming evidence for a true, devastating increase in autism, a surge that began with births in the late 1980s and has continued unabated.
From Figure 8, showing the exponential increase in the autism caseload in the California Developmental Services system, which is limited to cases of “developmental disability” type autism and omits most cases of high functioning autism.
By Jill Escher
When it comes to the question of autism trends over the past several decades, it is undisputed that the prevalence of diagnosed cases has grown dramatically, in the range of .01-.05% for children born in the 1950s to about 3% for children born more recently, at least in industrialized countries like the U.S., the U.K., Australia, South Korea and Japan [Figure 1].
While no one in the field seriously disputes these numbers, a debate continues to rage over whether this 60-fold increase is owing to an actual epidemic of abnormal brain development in our children, or a mere conceptual evolution resulting in re-labeling of mental conditions that have always been around, or a combination of these two factors.
Based on a reading of studies spanning seven decades and more than a dozen countries, reams of administrative data, as well as expert interviews, I can see only one resounding conclusion: the evidence is overwhelming for a true, torrential increase in autism, beginning with children born from the late 1980s and accelerating in an exponential manner through today. Even comparing apples to apples without incorporating a broader definition of autism into the scope of comparison, the data reflecting autism’s ascendance are Mt. Everest in scale compared to the molehills of the contrary arguments.
To explain, I’ll go over both the evidence for a true increase and the arguments against it. While it’s impossible to address all the many evidentiary threads I’ll try to hit on some highlights.
Evidence for a true increase
Let’s examine evidence for autism’s increasing rates through three basic modes of exploration, from the most casual to the most scrupulous: common-sense observation, administrative data, and epidemiological studies.
1. Common-sense observation
Observations may not amount to objective science but they are relevant to the overall picture and a worthy component of critical thinking that should considered in any discussion of public health trends. For example, it’s hard to miss that relative to today, few Americans were obese in the 1970s, based on collective memory and photographic/video evidence. While observations like these are hardly gold-standard evidence, reasonable people can observe such a trend and allow it feed into their truth algorithm, while at the same time being open to contrary evidence that may flow from careful research. If 3% of babies are born with blue hair, and we’re pretty sure we hadn’t seen that in generations past, we’re allowed to employ basic logic to conclude it’s highly likely a new phenomenon, but again subject to contrary evidence from carefully conducted studies.
With that in mind, let’s run an observational logic experiment based on the overview of the explosion in childhood autism prevalence seen in Figure 1. According to the latest U.S. Centers for Disease Control (CDC) Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring (ADDM) network data, among U.S. children with autism an estimated 35.2% also have intellectual disability (ID) (IQ≤70), 23.1% have borderline ID (IQ 71-85), and 41.7% have average or higher IQ (IQ>85). For the sake of argument, let’s put aside the 41.7% with average or higher IQ as possibly going without any label in the past (which is unlikely, but again, for the sake of the thought experiment). That leaves 58.3% of cases that would likely not have been overlooked in the past. Now look at Figure 2. If there has been no true increase in autism, we would have to believe practitioners and researchers missed all the green cases of autism in the 1950s. That amounts to about 1.75% of all children, less 0.05% who were identified, for a total of 1.7% of all children that no one noticed were substantially disabled by autism (or any related label). Is this remotely possible? We are allowed to be highly skeptical that throughout the modern era that skilled practitioners missed such a massive number of affected children.
In the same vein, if today’s autism rates existed in the 1940s, we should be questioning whether it’s possible that in his landmark 1943 paper first describing autism, renowned child psychiatrist Leo Kanner could have characterized his 11 novel cases as something “markedly and uniquely different from anything reported so far” — and also see none of his peers laughing at his assertion. Again, even if we are to limit autism just to cases with ID or borderline ID, that would be 1.75% of all children. Did the entire field of psychiatry miss seeing observably disabling autism in 1.75% of all children? This is a preposterous notion. The most rational take on the situation is that Kanner and the psychiatric profession considered autism to be extremely rare because back then, it was. Even in the 1970s, when researcher Lorna Wing insisted on an expanded view of autism, she contended autism was not about 0.04% as thought in the U.K. as found by careful field studies, but maybe four times higher, at least in the London area. However, Dr. Wing’s maximum of .16% still falls extremely short of today’s 3%, or even the stricter-definition 1.75%.
We are often told that children with autism existed in these numbers but were hiding under other labels, such as “feeble-minded,” or later, “childhood schizophrenia” or “mental retardation,” or that they were hidden away in institutions and left uncounted. But there is no evidence for these contentions.
The U.S. Census of 1890 found 95,609 “feeble-minded” persons of a population of 62,979,766, for a total of .15% of the population. It was later estimated that about 55,000 individuals had not been counted, but that would barely move the prevalence rate. It is interesting to note that the Scotland census of 1891 found a similar rate; just 0.125% of the population were “imbeciles.”
As for old diagnostic labels, the historic rate of childhood schizophrenia was vanishingly low and could not come close to accounting for the high rate of autism today. For example, a landmark study of U.S. children born in the early 1960s, the multi-site Collaborative Perinatal Project (CPP) study found only 6 out of 30,000, or .02% of children evaluated for mental health outcomes had childhood schizophrenia or related mental disorders. A 1970 study by Donald Treffert found even lower rates of childhood schizophrenia among children in Wisconsin. As for the argument that a diagnostic shift from Mental Retardation/Intellectual Disability explains escalating rates of autism, that will be addressed later in this essay.
Regarding institutions, we often hear that rates of autism were low in the past because such people were hidden away in institutions. In fact, in 2014 I met with an autism clinician named Bennett Leventhal at his office at UCSF. He crossed his arms and said, “I assure you, Jill, there has been no true increase in autism,” and then proceeded to sketch on his whiteboard a house with stick figures both on the bottom floor and in the attic. The kids in the metaphorical attic just weren’t counted, he argues. But Dr. Leventhal’s view does not comport with reality. The percentage of people with autism sent to institutions was extremely small compared to the 3% prevalence we see today. California’s Department of Developmental Services (CalDDS) counts about 190,000 cases of developmental-disability level autism today, but in 1964, at the height of the period of institutionalization in the state, there were about 13,000 people with developmental disabilities residing in the developmental centers, and another 3,000 on waiting lists. Keep in mind the 13,000 was composed of a diverse population with Down syndrome, cerebral palsy, epilepsy, brain injury, ID, and genetic disorders, in addition to autism. Autism would have been at the absolute highest possible estimate a few thousand cases. Today’s CalDDS autism population dwarves any possible earlier institutionalized population, even accounting for the doubling of the state population over that time. The CPP data are also illuminating. Of the CPP’s 40,000 children born in the early 1960s, only about 125 children were institutionalized, for total of .31% of children. As with the California institutions, those would have comprised a wide variety of mental disorders, not just autism. And going back in time to the U.S. 1890 Census, only 5,254 of the enumerated 95,609 feeble-minded individuals were in institutions for the feeble-minded and 2,469 were in insane asylums.
The most obvious observational logic question, though, is this: if autism has always been around, and at these rates, we would see a commensurately high rate of senior citizens with autism in group homes, day programs, hospitals or other services or accommodations. But we don’t. There is not a single eyewitness account or data source, anywhere, showing anything remotely like a 3% autism rate among seniors. Community observations on this point are worthy of consideration, and here’s one. Sue Swezey, a mother of a son with autism and a co-founder of Autism Society San Francisco Bay Area, shared in a blogpost that her son John, now 61 years old, was born in 1963 (back when studies found autism rates to be about 0.01-0.05%). She says in the 1960s and 70s that John “was a little boy very much alone with his diagnosis,” that people “had never seen anyone like him before,” and that nobody doubted the research-established prevalence rate. John’s first and second school districts both counted only one other child with autism (these districts now have hundreds of students with autism sufficiently severe to qualify for special education). There were so few autism families that when advocating at the state capitol the handful of autism parents resorted to wigs and wardrobe changes to appear more numerous. I should also add that I’ve known John for years and he is considerably higher functioning that my own two children with autism and hundreds others I know just here in our region, so I find it ridiculous to think that innumerable cases even more severe than John were overlooked.
John is now a senior citizen with few autistic peers his age. Based on data from the CalDDS, as of 2016 the entire statewide system counted just 146 clients with autism born in his birth year of 1963. Please compare that to the 5,163 CalDDS autism cases born in 2010. Where are the thousands of missing autistic 61 year-olds like John? There is no evidence they exist; the CalDDS data doesn’t even have them showing up as “ever” in the system, not just active cases. If one is going to argue an “always been here” position, one should at least offer some data demonstrating this torrent of allegedly overlooked cases, whether from psychiatric, school, military, hospital, welfare or other systems. But no one has ever been able to produce such data.
Community service providers are also a trove of historical wisdom. Having spoken with more than a dozen developmental disability providers in the San Francisco Bay Area over the years, their chorus is also uniform: for decades they had a steady caseload of Down syndrome, other genetic disorders, cerebral palsy, brain injury, and intellectual disability, and occasionally, autism. Then in the 1990s they saw a rapid upswing in the numbers of children with autism, and in the 2000s they began noticing a strong surge of young adults aging out of the school system, not just with the label of autism but also with qualitatively different characteristics of autism, such as limited language, weak social skills, cognitive inflexibility, and disruptive and dangerous behaviors. The new crop of young adults with autism were more challenging and costly to serve, an observation backed up by CalDDS expenditure data. The service providers struggled. The budgets and staff could not keep up, a trend seen across the nation, as the providers helplessly witness an unprecedented annual “firehose of autistic 21 year-olds” (as one New Jersey housing provider recently put it) desperately needing adult programs. Alex Krem, whose family has run camps and recreational programs for children and adults with developmental disabilities in Northern California since 1961, hardly saw any autism cases until the 1990s, and now the program’s entrants are mostly child and young adult autism cases.
Autism is now constantly in the news, something we take for granted. But autism hardly made any headlines before the 1990s. Now we hear almost weekly about a child with autism who has wandered and drowned, a now-common tragedy somehow not noticed in news reporting before this century. We hear about new autism schools continually opening, when no one saw much need for these schools before the 1990s. We see story after story of families crying out for help, while officials say they are overwhelmed and can’t keep up with crushing waitlists. We see sensory hours at stores, sensory-friendly shows, autism parking spots, the need for enhanced police training, specially designed parks. Is this due to mere awareness, or a punishing burden of need? It seems impossible to me that the entire news industry missed reporting on this population for the entire history of our nation until the 1990s.
(As someone who has battled against the debunked vaccine hypothesis of autism, I am loath to refer to a website authored by one of its proponents, but we should give credit where credit it due. A website by Anne Dachel catalogues over 9,000 news stories relating to the growing rates of autism, especially in the schools. Though her suggested reason for autism’s ascendance is clearly erroneous, she is a helpful shepherd of an avalanche of stories reflecting the dire impact of the torrential and unrelenting increase of autism.)
Personally, I never saw or heard of a single person with autism, except the fictitious Rain Main, throughout my youth and young adulthood, until the diagnosis of my son Jonathan, born in 1999. When we sought a class for Jonny through our school district, the staff commented on the convulsive growth in autism they noticed in the recent years, an onslaught that seemed to come from nowhere. Jonny’s pediatrician, who had seen thousands of children over a long career, had not seen much autism either, under any label. I hear the same refrain from countless other old-timers in education and clinical services. A speech therapist friend explained how autism was barely part of her training curriculum in the 1980s because so few patients with that profile were ever seen. Even the former director of the National Institute for Mental Health (NIMH), Dr. Thomas Insel, remarked: “When my brother trained at Children’s Hospital at Harvard in the 1970s, they admitted a child with autism, and the head of the hospital brought all of the residents through to see. He said, ‘You’ve got to see this case; you’ll never see it again.'”
The no-true-increase adherents are asking us to engage in collective amnesia. Not only are we allowed to invoke common sense observations about today’s autism population eclipsing that of previous generations by orders of magnitude, we absolutely must if we are to seek the truth. But now we are now barely off the starting blocks. Let’s move on to administrative data.
(2) Administrative data
Administrative data is a big step above casual observation. It generally refers to data kept by organizations and agencies that are tasked with providing some sort of service or keeping track of cases in a registry. Such data can come from many sources, including hospital and insurance systems, school districts, and safety net systems such as social security disability programs and state Medicaid programs. This data is usually limited to raw overall caseload or caseload by birth cohort, but sometimes is subjected to further analysis to derive birth year, or birth cohort, case count or prevalence.
Hospital systems. Let’s start with hospitals. The largest U.S. study of autism prevalence seen in hospital systems is via the nation-wide Mental Health Research Network (MHRN). Figure 3 shows the MHRN autism diagnosis rate grows higher with each birth cohort, with the exception of ages 0-4, which is likely explained by a usual lag in diagnosis. Autism in adults over age of 35 is shown to be extremely rare. Autism rates are about 2.8-3.1% for children aged 5-8 in 2022, consistent with the rate seen in the CDC ADDM network and other federal studies. Did these 12 large hospital systems all miss massive numbers of adults with autism? It seems unlikely to the point of impossible.
Schools. Schools in the U.S. keep robust autism data due to the 1975 federal special education law (now called the Individuals with Disabilities in Education Act, or IDEA) mandating a free and appropriate public education for pupils with disabilities, including autism. The mandate only applies if the autism negatively affects the child’s educational performance and therefore typically does not cover the very mild cases; an autism case is included in special education data if the child requires extra supports or services beyond those in the general curriculum. Even given this limitation, autism cases in special ed have skyrocketed.
In California, the number of students with autism exploded, from 14,038 in the 2000-01 school year to an astounding 167,341 in 2023-24. That’s a 12-fold increase over 23 years, at a time of generally declining school enrollment in the state. Though it is often speculated that this surge can be explained by a diagnostic shift out of the ID category, the data show this to be untrue. The ID numbers remained flat over that time, around 40,000 for both of the those school years [Figure 4]. From a prevalence, as opposed to raw case count, perspective over a shorter period of time, just ten years between 2011 and 2020, we again see an autism surge while ID and other related categories held steady [Figure 5].
This same trend is seen in other states. For example, an older study was conducted in Minnesota on children aged 6 to 11 years by James Gurney et al. using a special education definition of autism. ASD cases had surged exponentially in the state beginning in the 1991 school year [Figure 6]. The researchers found ASD prevalence in this age bracket increased from 0.03% in 1991-1992 to 0.52% in 2001-2002, a 17-fold increase, while the other special educational disability categories also increased during this period, except for mild mental handicap, which decreased only slightly. With respect to the huge surge of ASD, the authors remarked that “diagnostic substitution does not largely explain the increasing trends.”
In 2019, Massachusetts reported that its schools were serving four times as many students with autism as it did 15 years prior. Just recently Wyoming reported a 60% increase in autistic children in early childhood education and a 19% increase in autistic children in K-12 schools, just since 2018. We can see reports like this from every state.
Nationwide data shows that whereas less than 5% of disabled students in the U.S. had autism in 2008-2009, just 14 years later, by 2022-23, that number had jumped to nearly 13%. Based on somewhat earlier numbers from 49 states, the number of 6- to 21-year-old students classified as having autism rose 165% between the 2005-06 and 2014-15 school years.
Like the U.S., the U.K. schools have also experienced an enormous growth in autism cases. A 2020 report by Roy McConkey found massive increases in autism over nine years from 2010-11 to 2018-19 in schools in all four U.K. countries, noting that “the rate of increase shows little sign of abating and may indeed be accelerating.” Northern Ireland had the highest prevalence, reaching 3.2%. Just recently the Telegraph reported that the cost of supporting schoolchildren with special needs has jumped again by more than two-thirds since just 2018-19, from £6.9bn to £12bn, paralleling the increase in the number of students with autism.
In Australia, about 4% of seven- to 14-year-old students now have a primary diagnosis of autism, while between 6% and 10% of children have ADHD. As a consequence of the unprecedented numbers of students needing specialized supports, “Parents, teachers and advocates say education is at crisis point.”
Medicaid and SSI. In the U.S., the Medicaid system is the largest provider of adult supports such as personal assistance, habilitative day programs, and disability services in residential facilities such as group homes. Entry into this system varies somewhat by state but overall one can only obtain Medicaid developmental disability services where the state finds you suffer such a substantial functional disability that you are incapable of achieving many basics of daily living. Disability-based Medicaid is not available those who are merely quirky or who think differently. Even in this restricted system, comparing apples to apples, we see a dramatic surge in autism rates in younger birth cohorts, with prevalence of Medicaid autism cases more than doubling over 8 years for the 18-24 age group [Figure 7].
Similarly, the federal Supplemental Security Income system, which provides monthly welfare payments to income-qualifying children and adults with autism and other disabilities experienced a similar growth in autism cases. Adults with ASD represented a growing share of the total first-time SSI awards given to adults with mental disorders, with percentages increasing from 1.3% in 2005 to 5.0% in 2015. In 2015, 158,105 adults with ASD received SSI benefits, a 326.8% increase over 2005. Federal SSI payments to adults with ASD increased by 383.2% during the same period (totaling roughly $1.0 billion in 2015).
State Medicaid programs: California Department of Developmental Services. Finally, let’s look at the data from CalDDS, which is well known to be among the nation’s most comprehensive, both in terms of time horizon and case capture. This is due both to the state’s unique statute, the Lanterman Act, which has long mandated case-finding of and services for residents with developmental disabilities, as well as the state’s expansive population, now at 39 million, about the same as the entire country of Canada, and representing nearly 12% of the entire U.S. population.
Through the 1980s, the autism caseload was modest, edging up from about 3,000 cases. But in the 1990s the system saw a tremendous unexpected influx of cases, and the legislature grew alarmed. After commissioning a 1999 study that confirmed the reality of the increase, which could not be explained as a diagnostic artifact, by 2003 the state legislature enacted a rule tightening CalDDS eligibility requirements. This attempt to stop the tsunami of cases proved futile however, as ever-more autism cases with seriously disabling symptoms grew year over year [Figure 8]. Between 1989 and April 2024, the autism caseload grew nearly 60-fold, or 6,000%, while the state population grew only about 33% larger. When the U.C. Davis MIND Institute determined the CalDDS autism prevalence rate by birth year, it saw a dramatic increase from birth year 1980 to 2014 [Figure 9].
While some contend that this tremendous growth in autism cases is due to a parallel decline in ID cases in CalDDS, the numbers do not bear this out [Figure 10]. ID cases grew while autism cases also climbed. Finally, the enormous transformation in the composition of the CalDDS caseload cannot be missed, either in the trenches or in the data, with autism growing from a tiny slice of the developmental disability population in 1993 to about half in 2023 [Figure 11].
Overall, the data from our nation’s most populous state paints a dire picture. Autism cases have been rising dramatically since births in the 1980s, despite the enactment of stricter entry requirements in 2003. Because of the requirement that each autism case involve substantial functional limitations across three major life activities, CalDDS autism represents only the more severely affected of all the state’s autism cases. There is not a shred of over-diagnosis here, or in Medicaid or SSI. Yet even these subsets have exploded in numbers, a phenomenon that has not and cannot be explained by diagnostic shifts, awareness or broadening of any slight shifts in psychiatric criteria.
(3) Epidemiological data
While the observational and administrative evidence may be strong and consistent, these data are not considered as bullet-proof and reliable as numbers from epidemiological studies conducted by experts and which involve active case review, fieldwork and/or case-finding. So let’s comb through what the gold-standard studies have shown. Do they deviate from what we have already seen? No, they only strengthen the case. We’ll start with studies in the U.S. and then turn to international numbers.
U.S. Epidemiology. The first large-scale epidemiological study on autism in the U.S. took place across the state of Wisconsin in the mid-1960s, ascertaining autism in children aged 12. In a 1970 paper Donald Treffert and his colleagues found a prevalence rate of 0.01% (1 in 10,000). Critics contend that this low prevalence only reflected what we would today call profound autism, but let us note the obvious. The CDC ADDM data on the 2012 birth cohort pinned Wisconsin’s autism prevalence at 2.8%. Even if, for the sake of argument, we limit that 2.8% to the estimated 26.7% of overall autism cases that rise to the level of profound autism (the 26.7% of autism as profound autism proportion comes from a CDC study to be discussed below), that’s a prevalence of profound autism of 0.74% in Wisconsin’s 8 year-olds, which is 74-fold higher than the autism rate of the 1960s. Did Treffert and colleagues miss about 74 cases for each 1 they found? It’s simply not possible.
Around the same time as the Wisconsin study, the aforementioned Collaborative Perintal Project, or CPP, found an autism rate of about 0.046% in the 30,000 seven year-old children who underwent mental health evaluations. The CPP was particularly thorough in finding even subtle cases that the educational and medical systems would not have captured. In a paper by Torrey et al. the 14 children with autism, three had IQ scores above 70 (71, 72, 73), four had scores below 70 (37, 45, 50 and 50), and seven were considered untestable. The paper also identified six additional CPP children who were labeled as severely disturbed, apparently psychotic, childhood schizophrenic or possibly autistic. Four had abnormal language development and abnormal affective human contact when younger, but at age 7 had normal neurological development. The other two children were said to be obviously abnormal. The IQs in this group were 54, 67, 78, 82, and two were not testable. So, for the sake of constructing an even more conservative comparison to autism today, let’s add all those into the CPP autism pile, for a total of 20 children out of 30,000, or a rate of 0.066% cases. If we label those as classic autism (again, they clearly were not in some cases), this puny rate is just a speck compared to today’s rate of profound autism, which is at least .5% of all children. The CPP data also do not suggest that autism cases were hidden in the MR category, as about 1% of the children were found to have MR, which is even lower than ID rates seen today.
In 1984-88, Edward Ritvo and colleagues undertook an epidemiological study of childhood autism in Utah. The group found 241 of 483 potential cases had autism, for a prevalence of .04%, and this included not just the most severe cases, as one-third had IQs greater than 70. To see if changing diagnostic schemes might increase these low rates, a 2013 study by Miller et al. re-ascertained the potential cases under DSM-IV, finding 64 additional children originally “Diagnosed Not Autistic” met the new ASD case definition, with the average IQ estimate in the newly identified group significantly lower than in the original group. The inclusion of new cases would not have significantly altered the .04% prevalence, however.
Incidentally, the CDC ADDM data on 2012 birth cohort estimated Utah’s autism prevalence at 2.2%. Even if we limit for the sake of argument the 2.2% to the estimated 26.7% of cases with profound autism, that’s a prevalence of profound autism of 0.59% in Utah’s 8 year-olds, which is 15-fold higher than the fuller-spectrum autism rate detected in the 1980s.
Around the same time, a 1987 paper by Larry Burd and colleagues ascertained an autism rate of 0.033% in children in North Dakota (59 of 180,000 children). Of those, 21 met DSM-III criteria for infantile autism (.012%) and the remainder had more broadly defined pervasive developmental disorders (PDD). I need not explain the difference between 0.033% in the 1980s compared to the 3.0% today.
CDC Prevalence Studies. After years of the unexpected and dramatic increases in autism seen in clinics across the country, the CDC launched its first systematic childhood autism prevalence studies as part of the ADDM network, with 2000 as its first surveillance year examining children born in 1992 and who were then 8 years old. Ten additional ADDM cycles have been published, all employing fairly exacting methodologies in multiple different sites across the United States. The longitudinal prevalence results are summarized in Figure 12, with the most recent rate being 2.67% in 8 year-old children born in the year 2012 and ascertained in 2020 (another report from the ADDM will be released in March 2025).
As discussed above, an estimated 35.2% also have intellectual disability (IQ≤70), 23.1% have borderline intellectual disability (IQ 71-85), and 41.7% have average or higher IQ (IQ≥ 85). While it is true that rates of autism with Q≥ 85 have increased faster than the other two categories, one can easily see the sharp increases in autism in the other two, lower IQ, groups over time [Figure 13].
Nonetheless, all autism cases in the ADDM must meet clinical standards for autism, and case definition has been held constant over time. In the words of epidemiologist Walter Zahorodny, PhD, who has studied autism rates for a quarter century and serves a lead investigator for the ADDM site in New Jersey, autism under the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) and often used in epidemiology “means an individual’s overall life functioning in practical circumstances is impaired or disabled, they have some significant deficit in being able to do expected things of their age.” He continues, “We never participated in the program as looking for autistic features or signs or personality characteristics—there are other disorders in the DSM which have elements of similarity to autism but not autism. Autism is not atypical social preference, but children who are easily recognized by experts as having a disorder.”
It’s important to note that at the end of 2019 the CDC shifted its approach to examine only already-diagnosed autism cases, rather than active case-finding which includes unidentified cases. The CDC moved to this less comprehensive system to improve speed and efficiency, and to respond to decreased funding allocated to autism surveillance and research. The 2016 cycle might be the last one to find as many cases as possible, not just those already diagnosed in the community. Nevertheless, despite the more lax methods, the ADDM autism numbers continued to climb in both the 2018 and 2020 surveillance years. In the 2020 surveillance the startling increase may partly be due to the inclusion of a California site in northern San Diego County finding an unusually high autism prevalence of 4.5% 8 year-olds. But on the other hand, the overall ADDM prevalence estimate was 2.76%, which is less than the prevalence reported in other federal studies. The CDC’s National Health Interview Survey (NHIS), a comprehensive health survey of U.S. families, found that for children in 2022, 3.79% had been identified with autism. The 2019-2021 Health Resources and Services Administration (HRSA) National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH) indicated higher a rate as well, finding about 3.1% had been diagnosed as having autism.
It is often repeated that over the course of its existence the CDC ADDM found an increase in autism prevalence growing from .67% in surveillance year 2000 (birth year 1992), to 2.76% for year 2020 (birth year 2012). But it’s worth noting that the CDC conducted a study on a 1988 birth cohort as well, as part of its Metropolitan Atlanta Developmental Disabilities Surveillance Program (MADDSP) (Van Naarden Braun et al 2015), using similar methods. The researchers found prevalence for birth year 1988 to be .42%. When the MADDSP prevalence grew to 1.5% in the 2002 birth cohort (which was comparable to the rate found in the larger ADDM study), the authors only found plausible ID-to-ASD diagnostic substitution for non-hispanic black females, a small subset of the autism population. Figure 14 shows the tremendous growth in autism while other disabilities remained flat or, in the case of ID, grew a moderate amount.
Finally, one must also consider that the CDC conducted a study to ascertain the prevalence of profound autism, which restricts the case definition to the most stringent criteria: typically involving an IQ under 50 and the need for round-the-clock care or supervision. Even with this intense restriction, we still see very substantial prevalence growth. Over just 16 years, 2000-2026, the prevalence of profound autism nearly doubled, from 0.27% of 8 year-old children to 0.46% [Figure 15]. And as mentioned before, 26.7% of all the 8 year-olds with autism were estimated to suffer from profound autism.
New Jersey epidemiology. Perhaps more than children in any other state, New Jersey children have been the subject of rigorous autism epidemiology over the past two decades, both as part of the ADDM and other efforts. The 2020 ADDM surveillance of 8 year-olds found an overall prevalence of 2.9%, up from 0.9% two decades prior, a surge seen across all forms of autism and all demographic groups. Additional studies have added troubling nuance to these findings. In some counties, autism affected 5% to 7% of the pediatric population, including 5% in Ocean County and 7% in Toms River, based on 2022 reports. Dr. Zahorodny stresses that the same ascertainment strategies have been used to validate true autism cases across all studies. Despite the staggering growth in autism, about a quarter of cases in children and adolescents are un- or mis-diagnosed in the community. Even as late as 2016, one of five 8 year-olds who met criteria for autism did not have a community diagnosis. Among 16 year-olds as many as a quarter did not have a diagnosis. Minority or low SES cases are more likely to be undiagnosed or misdiagnosed, and they receive their first professional evaluation later than white children.
From 2000-2010, across many studies in New Jersey, Zahorodny and colleagues found a strong positive association between wealth and autism prevalence. However, since 2010 they have found that this wealth gradient is reversed, finding greater rates in low- and middle-income communities. In the study of adolescents, 16 years old, 59% had other co-occurring neuropsychiatric disorders, 35% had ID, and one in four cases had been undiagnosed or misdiagnosed. ASD among adolescents was two times as common in high-income compared to low-income communities. Black and Hispanic more likely to have ID. In a sign of the stability of their methodology over time, autism prevalence estimates found at 8 years old were the same for the cohort at 16 years old.
International epidemiology. To explore data from outside the U.S., let’s start with studies from across the pond. The earliest studies of autism from the U.K. showed a prevalence of about 0.01-0.05% among children. Victor Lotter’s 1966 study of children aged 8-10 years attending schools in England’s Middlesex county in the mid- 1960s found that 0.045% percent had autism, with only half of that group showing “nuclear autism,” meeting the criteria for Kanner autism, which perhaps is comparable to profound autism today. One-third had had IQ above 55. Wing and Gould's 1979 study looked for children with social impairment in Camberwell, London in the 1970s. Among this broader group, using the same criteria as Lotter’s study, the authors found 0.049% had autism, with slightly less than half of these showing “nuclear autism.”
Studies in the 1980s found a slightly higher prevalence of about 0.077%, using a broader definition of autism based on DSM-III, then the DSM-III-R criteria. But in the 1990s, things started going haywire, with a likely 0.1 to 0.12% rate, as reported by Gillberg and Wing in 1999. Using the most expansive criteria, the authors suggested a potential prevalence of 0.4% to 0.5% might even be possible. Successive studies have found steadily increasing prevalence since that time, up to 2023 when a study of children in England born 2004-2008 yielded a prevalence of 2.94%.
Moving beyond the northern border, Canada has not conducted longitudinal epidemiological investigations like the ADDM, but various studies reflect a trend similar to that in the U.S. In Canada, autism prevalence grew from 0.12% for birth years 1987-1991, to 2.16% in a study concerning birth year 2006.
In Denmark, children born in about 1980 had a .01% autism prevalence, but a more recent study from 2018 found a 2.8% rate. A 2015 study finding an increase in autism back in the very earliest years of the autism surge, births 1980-1991, found that a change in diagnostic criteria in 1994 and the inclusion of outpatient diagnoses in 1995 (specific to Denmark) accounted for perhaps 40% of increase over that specific period of time. There’s no indication in more recent rates, including the 2.8% in the 2018 study, are owing to diagnostic shifts. Consistently higher autism rates in each birth cohort from 1990 to 2007 were seen not just in Denmark but also in Sweden, Finland and Western Australia. In Iceland, children born between 1994 and 1998 and followed until 2009, had an autism prevalence of 1.2%, but children born approximately 2006-2008 had a sharply higher prevalence of 3.13%.
Israel has the advantage of massive nation-wide health and benefits insurance databases which epidemiologists have used to track prevalence over time. Most recently, Dinstein et al. 2024 reported a rate of 1.56% for children born in 2013. This is a large increase from estimates of 0.3% in 2008, 0.65% in 2015 and 1.3% in 2018. A 2015 study on the cumulative incidence of ASD child disability benefits in Israel, at age 8, for birth years 1992 to 2003, shows a skyrocketing increase in autism disability claims versus modest growth for other disabilities. A 2020 study by Davidovitch et al., finding the unprecedented 1.8% rate, stated that “changes affecting diagnostic criteria for ASD and eligibility for ASD-related services did not appreciably affect these trends.”
Japan has also witnessed climbing autism rates. For children born in 1988, prevalence was 0.21%, with about half of children having IQs of 70 and above. In more recent years, the cumulative incidence for each birth cohort showed a steady increase in autism, from 2.23% for the 2009 birth cohort to 3.26% for the 2014 birth cohort.
Interestingly, lower autism rates have been detected outside of industrialized countries. For example, a study of children in rural Nepal 2001-2004 yielded 0.3%, and 1.11% for children in parts of India born 2000-2010.
Arguments against a true increase
Change of coding systems. While people often invoke changes in coding systems as a factor driving increasing rates of diagnosis, a large study found a shift to the ICD-10 (International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision), which the U.S. adopted in 2015, did not meaningfully affect autism rates at ten large healthcare systems. The rates of autism were increasing for 0- to 5-year-olds before the transition to the ICD-10 and did not substantively change after the new coding was in place. Similarly, in South Korea, changing from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, DSM-IV to DSM-V led to a decrease in autism prevalence. An intensive community case-finding study found found 2.64% childhood ASD prevalence under DSM IV. The number decreased by 17% to 2.2% under DSM V.
Studies. Proponents of the no-increase hypothesis often trot out a smattering of studies to support the idea that autism has always been here at today’s high rates. Let’s look at some.
A 2011 paper and 2016 report by Traolach Brugha, a professor at the University of Leicester, England are often invoked to counter the evidence for rising prevalence. The 2011 paper looked for autism in persons aged 16 and older residing in a small area of England, around the University, using a convoluted experimental self-report methodology and not gold-standard case ascertainment. The study concluded an autism prevalence of 1% for age 16 and older, based on just 19 cases.
The 2016 Report also came with a disclaimer: “Statistics relating to autism in this report are experimental” with methods “validated” only by Brugha and his team. The report estimated prevalence of adult autism at 0.8%, with a wide confidence interval. This was based on just 31 cases from combined 2007 and 2014 samples. Even the report noted it was “small for subgroup analysis and means caution with interpretation is required.” It also had red-flag questionable findings, such as: “Among 16 to 64 year olds, employment status was not significantly related to whether or not someone was identified with autism.” This is odd because it’s well established that adults with autism have poor employment outcomes relative to typically developing individuals.
Needless to say, even putting aside the questionable methodology, the 1% or 0.8% prevalence found is far lower than the nearly 3% prevalence reported in children. Most risibly, even Brugha’s 2016 report found increasing birth year prevalence, with much higher rates in the 16-34 bracket than those 35 and older [Figure 16]. Even his own work confirmed a longitudinal increase in autism prevalence. As an aside, I can recall attending a session at the International Society for Autism Research meeting in Austin, Texas in 2022, when Dr. John Constantino, then of Washington University, refuted a question about growing rates of autism by pointing to the Brugha work, saying the question “has been asked and answered.” Horrified by his superficial reading of the reports, my jaw dropped to the floor but pretty much everyone else in the room nodded in agreement.
Related to the Brugha brouhaha, a frequently cited Nature Reviews Disease Primer published in 2020, viewed as a definitive consensus statement about autism, contained a brief section about autism prevalence authored by Dr. Brugha (per email to me from lead author Dr. Cathy Lord). It denies an increase in autism, stating there is “no clear evidence of a change in the prevalence of autism in the community between 1990 and 2010.” Of the six very cherry-picked studies cited for this proposition, three are to work by Brugha himself, and the other three studies offered no data to support the assertion of no increase. Nevertheless this sloppy and biased paragraph in a leading journal is sometimes construed as a sort of proof that autism prevalence has not increased. In reality it is scientific malpractice.
Another paper often cited is by Polyak et al. 2015, for the idea that autism’s increase was only caused by diagnostic shifts away from ID to ASD. The paper was not an epidemiological study on autism increase over time but instead intended to look for hints of common molecular origin for comorbid conditions. It was conducted by geneticists, not epidemiologists using proper tools of that trade. Instead of focusing on birth year prevalence, this study performed a clumping of all ages 3-21, which is meaningless to determine autism rates over time. It ended with a 0.52% autism prevalence in 2010 (a meaningless number because it's not based on birth year, as one sees with CDC ADDM for example). In any event, the study found that a large percent of the autism increase was not accounted for by declining ID, and many states did not reflect this trend at all, including the most populous states, California and Texas. Furthermore, the data is now 14 years old, and autism rates have clearly continued surging over that time. Moreover, the federal Office of Special Education Programs (OSEP) shows a dramatic increase in autism in special eduction, to 909,055 in 2022-23, up from 513,688 in 2014-15. Meanwhile OSEP data shows ID cases have flatlined, not decreased, 420,000 in 2022-23 from 415,335 in 2014-15.
A recent and sadly oft-cited U.S. study that attempted to ascertain adult prevalence was conducted only as a modeling experiment, led by the CDC. This paper however, did nothing more than take current childhood rates and project them onto older ages, without attempting a single stab at actual case-finding. This woefully superficial analysis is clearly lethally infected by a flawed, baseless set of assumptions about autism prevalence being steady over time, and must be, in my view, one of the most misleading papers in the history of autism.
A similarly dubious approach was taken in England by O’Nions et al. Their 2023 paper found rates of diagnosed autism in children/young people were much higher than in adults/older adults. As of 2018, 2.94% of 10- to 14-year-olds (born approx 2004-2008) had a diagnosis (1 in 34), vs. 0.02% aged 70+ (1 in 6,000). That’s a 147-fold increase. Then the researchers took an enormous leap of faith. They assumed a constant birth year prevalence over the decades to suggest that the data means that between 435,700 and 1,197,300 people in England may be autistic and undiagnosed. A ludicrous assumption with an equally ludicrous result.
Conclusion
We often hear that the autism increase is a mere artifact of changing conceptualizations but the data do not support that idea. The reasons people deny a reality of an autism epidemic are not scientific in nature but rather sociological.
First, with all good intentions both researchers and the media were hyper-cautious in reporting rising autism rates to defend against the debunked and dangerous hypothesis that childhood vaccinations cause autism. Second, with the strong evidence of autism’s heritability grew a rigid dogma that autism must be genetic in origin, and therefore could not be increasingly common. Though heritability may indeed be environmental in origin (this is the focus of my work as a research philanthropist), the faulty dogma has grown so strong that it cast a pall over all of epidemiology. Autism genetics seems to be a field that is impervious to negative feedback, but I believe that in the coming years the door will open to new ideas about autism’s origins and heritability.
Third, the rise of the neurodiversity ideology rested on some magical thinking that autism is more of a difference than a disorder, and saw autism more as an identify than a set of neurodevelopmental deficits. Adherents of this ideology simply could not accept that idea that autism rates were growing, a phenomenon seen in the popular revisionist history of autism by Steve Silberman, NeuroTribes (2015).
And as horrible as the autism surge might seem, the situation on the ground is actually much worse than what I’ve so far presented. The skyrocketing autism rates are just one part of a larger picture of mysteriously perturbed brain wiring in children in recent decades, as we see concurrent, if perplexing, surges in ADHD and other mental disorders like anxiety, depression, and gender dysphoria, all of which are alarming in themselves.
We can try to theorize it away, to obscure and downplay reality. We can try to rebrand the autism epidemic as an epidemic of awareness while wishful thinking rushes into fill an explanatory void. But mental gymnastics simply cannot explain the increase. We normalize at our peril this national emergency of obscene, unexplained rates of abnormal neurodevelopment.
Jill Escher is an autism research philanthropist (EscherFund.org), president of National Council on Severe Autism (ncsautism.org), past president of Autism Society San Francisco Bay Area (sfautismsociety.org) and that mother of two children with idiopathic profound autism. More info at jillescher.com.
_________________
"The reasonable man adapts himself to the world; the unreasonable one persists in trying to adapt the world to himself. Therefore all progress depends upon the unreasonable man."
- George Bernie Shaw
Yes, the evidence points to genuine, large increase in autism rates, which cannot be fully explained by changes in diagnostic criteria or diagnostic substitution.
It is frustrating that the increase tends to be brushed off as due to greater awareness, without research backing that up. There seems to be a blinkered approach to autism prevalence based on wishful thinking.
It deserves more consideration and further studies.
ASPartOfMe
Veteran
Joined: 25 Aug 2013
Age: 67
Gender: Male
Posts: 35,980
Location: Long Island, New York
Jill Eischer was born in 1965, 8 years later then me. For the purpose of this opinion piece I am treating her as a contemporary of mine because awareness and understanding of Autism during our formative years did not change nearly enough to make comparisons between our formative years and now irrelevant. Also I will be using the terms symptoms and not traits because diagnosing is by definition a part of the medical model of autism and thus the word clinicians would have used.
She's basing her theory by comparing prior studies with current ones. This is flawed for the same reasons retro diagnosing historical figures is flawed. Expanding criteria is the one most talked about. Comparing any studies that are based on different criteria is not scientific. She talks about the low percentage of autistics in institutions. She notes that there are no studies proving there was hidden or misdiagnosed autistics. This does not prove there was not misdiagnosed autistics in these institutions. Without valid comparisons as flawed as it is all we can go on is common sense which says stricter criteria means less diagnoses. Another reason comparing studies from different eras is flawed is that the meaning of language evolves. The clinicians then may have used a different words or terms to describe the same symptom. Also the same word may have had a different or more likely similar but not the same meanings. The clinicians of the 1970s were operating with different assumptions and contexts.
“I did not see any autistics when I was growing up” and the like is the weakest type of circumstantial evidence which Ms. Escher as a lawyer by trade surely knows. By definition we did not see peers whom were institutionalized. Not every person whose parents could not handle them were institutionalized some were locked in the attic, thrown in the street, or even killed. We did not see them either.
Peers we probably did see were not recognized because they were the undiagnosed and we were literally ignorant about autism. We saw them but described them as weird, painfully shy, various homophobic slurs, stubborn, attention seeker, and said they had a stick up their ass, etc. I knew of one such unrecognized person, I saw him in the mirror every morning.
For awhile I viewed both the autism epidemic and over-diagnoses explanations as both offensive and wrong. The expanding criteria theory seemed to explain it all.
While I still believe expanding criteria is the primary explanation I have made room for others.
I believe that while under diagnosis is still a problem for my peers over diagnosis is one for the youngest set. There is a huge push to diagnose as early as possible. We know the formation of “brain wiring” is at its most intense in the earliest part of life. ABA is intended to block or deflect the “autism genes” from wiring the brain before it becomes autistic. It is also good for the bottom line. Also due to search engines and now AI people have been trained to expect instant answers.
When people say environmental factors what they usually mean is environmental poisoning. There other possible environmental factors. Our senses are bombarded to a much larger degree then they were. Sights and sounds are not the only things bombarding our senses. The Free Range Parents of 1974 expected their kids to be home for dinner and do their homework. Today’s parents often hover over their children and plan every minute of their lives.
The parents of 1974 expected their children to figure out and deal with a lot more problems then todays patents do. For Level 2 and especially Level 3 autistics Free Range was a disaster, they need the structure and high level of guidance. For Level 1’s having the time and space to figure out what works and what does not for them was helped them figure out to some degree who they are and toughened them up. It was not a panacea. Learning the hard way is one thing but often this was done to a degree that caused unnecessary pain and lingering effects.
Today’s social expectations are higher.
If one could transfer todays diagnostic criteria and availability of diagnosis back to 1974 without changing anything else I contend the rate of autism diagnoses would be lower due to the factors I mentioned above. That does not mean there was less autism prevalence back then. It could mean certain peoples symptoms were not impairing enough for a diagnosis back then but due to said environmental factors are now.
Even if it is figured out through brain imaging or genetics what exactly autism is you are still going to run into issues comparing data from different eras.
_________________
Professionally Identified and joined WP August 26, 2013
DSM 5: Autism Spectrum Disorder, DSM IV: Aspergers Moderate Severity
“My autism is not a superpower. It also isn’t some kind of god-forsaken, endless fountain of suffering inflicted on my family. It’s just part of who I am as a person”. - Sara Luterman
It is frustrating that the increase tends to be brushed off as due to greater awareness, without research backing that up. There seems to be a blinkered approach to autism prevalence based on wishful thinking.
It deserves more consideration and further studies.
The fact that they've narrowed the criteria and there's still been an increase says that something is happening. (which is quite frankly massively frustrating to anybody that was sort of on the line as they moved the line without accounting for the folks that had nothing as an alternative diagnosis despite being autistic in everything except actual diagnosis)
ASPartOfMe
Veteran
Joined: 25 Aug 2013
Age: 67
Gender: Male
Posts: 35,980
Location: Long Island, New York
It is frustrating that the increase tends to be brushed off as due to greater awareness, without research backing that up. There seems to be a blinkered approach to autism prevalence based on wishful thinking.
It deserves more consideration and further studies.
The fact that they've narrowed the criteria and there's still been an increase says that something is happening. (which is quite frankly massively frustrating to anybody that was sort of on the line as they moved the line without accounting for the folks that had nothing as an alternative diagnosis despite being autistic in everything except actual diagnosis)
Not necessarily. At times when clinicians based on experience believe their client is autistic but technically does not fit the criteria, will write the diagnostic report in such a way as to be in complience.
_________________
Professionally Identified and joined WP August 26, 2013
DSM 5: Autism Spectrum Disorder, DSM IV: Aspergers Moderate Severity
“My autism is not a superpower. It also isn’t some kind of god-forsaken, endless fountain of suffering inflicted on my family. It’s just part of who I am as a person”. - Sara Luterman
It is frustrating that the increase tends to be brushed off as due to greater awareness, without research backing that up. There seems to be a blinkered approach to autism prevalence based on wishful thinking.
It deserves more consideration and further studies.
The fact that they've narrowed the criteria and there's still been an increase says that something is happening. (which is quite frankly massively frustrating to anybody that was sort of on the line as they moved the line without accounting for the folks that had nothing as an alternative diagnosis despite being autistic in everything except actual diagnosis)
Not necessarily. At times when clinicians based on experience believe their client is autistic but technically does not fit the criteria, will write the diagnostic report in such a way as to be in complience.
I'd wager that that's part of what's going on, because there are significant gaps that were created when they shrank the region. But, it's deeply problematic as whatever it is that succeeds the DSM 5 is likely to also remove the next closest diagnosis to AS and not put anything in there to replace it.
My main point there was that they shrank the officially recognized region of autism in an effort to cut down on the diagnoses being given out, but it hasn't done that and now there's just a bunch of sort of technically questionable diagnoses out there that make research and treatment a lot more complicated than it was.
Not sure what people mean by narrowing of criteria the opposite has happened has it not?
Autism merged with Asperger’s
Autism now is diagnosed into infinity
The only real evidence is Anecdotal.
There would be some clues however
Many special schools that existed in the 70’s and 80’s are still around however today about 70% of the children in them have autism
You would have to speak with borderline retired teachers from such schools that started out in the 80’s about changes they have seen between then and now
Were the kids more social then to now? What were their communication skills like?
was a two way conversation possible?
Did the 80’s kids have more of a learning disorder / adhd rather than the deep communication problems of autism?
_________________
"The reasonable man adapts himself to the world; the unreasonable one persists in trying to adapt the world to himself. Therefore all progress depends upon the unreasonable man."
- George Bernie Shaw
Autism merged with Asperger’s
Autism now is diagnosed into infinity
The only real evidence is Anecdotal.
There would be some clues however
Many special schools that existed in the 70’s and 80’s are still around however today about 70% of the children in them have autism
You would have to speak with borderline retired teachers from such schools that started out in the 80’s about changes they have seen between then and now
Were the kids more social then to now? What were their communication skills like?
was a two way conversation possible?
Did the 80’s kids have more of a learning disorder / adhd rather than the deep communication problems of autism?
The criteria are objectively narrower than under the DSM IV as a significant portion of the people that had qualified for diagnosis of autistic disorder or Asperger's Syndrome no longer qualify for diagnosis without relying on being grandfathered in. Not to mention that anybody who doesn't show clear traits early enough can't get PDD-NOS as a diagnosis to cover cases where the normal criteria weren't quite met for purely technical reasons.
AS was probably always something distinct from autism having as much effect in common with ScPD as with the autism diagnoses. Unfortunately, they couldn't be bothered to do the research and obliterated the diagnosis before such research could be performed.
Autism merged with Asperger’s
Autism now is diagnosed into infinity
The only real evidence is Anecdotal.
There would be some clues however
Many special schools that existed in the 70’s and 80’s are still around however today about 70% of the children in them have autism
You would have to speak with borderline retired teachers from such schools that started out in the 80’s about changes they have seen between then and now
Were the kids more social then to now? What were their communication skills like?
was a two way conversation possible?
Did the 80’s kids have more of a learning disorder / adhd rather than the deep communication problems of autism?
The criteria are objectively narrower than under the DSM IV as a significant portion of the people that had qualified for diagnosis of autistic disorder or Asperger's Syndrome no longer qualify for diagnosis without relying on being grandfathered in. Not to mention that anybody who doesn't show clear traits early enough can't get PDD-NOS as a diagnosis to cover cases where the normal criteria weren't quite met for purely technical reasons.
AS was probably always something distinct from autism having as much effect in common with ScPD as with the autism diagnoses. Unfortunately, they couldn't be bothered to do the research and obliterated the diagnosis before such research could be performed.
Great comment never really looked up Schizoid Personality Disorder, the similarities with Asperger's is interesting.
I would bet many self diagnosed may have ScPD, or even many diagnosed hf level 1 or aspies really have this, but claim or are diagnosed autistic in error, however both are really paper diagnosis with no real biology separating Asperger's or even many idiopathic autism with ScPD.
https://neurodivergentinsights.com/misd ... -vs-autism
_________________
"The reasonable man adapts himself to the world; the unreasonable one persists in trying to adapt the world to himself. Therefore all progress depends upon the unreasonable man."
- George Bernie Shaw
Autism merged with Asperger’s
Autism now is diagnosed into infinity
The only real evidence is Anecdotal.
There would be some clues however
Many special schools that existed in the 70’s and 80’s are still around however today about 70% of the children in them have autism
You would have to speak with borderline retired teachers from such schools that started out in the 80’s about changes they have seen between then and now
Were the kids more social then to now? What were their communication skills like?
was a two way conversation possible?
Did the 80’s kids have more of a learning disorder / adhd rather than the deep communication problems of autism?
The criteria are objectively narrower than under the DSM IV as a significant portion of the people that had qualified for diagnosis of autistic disorder or Asperger's Syndrome no longer qualify for diagnosis without relying on being grandfathered in. Not to mention that anybody who doesn't show clear traits early enough can't get PDD-NOS as a diagnosis to cover cases where the normal criteria weren't quite met for purely technical reasons.
AS was probably always something distinct from autism having as much effect in common with ScPD as with the autism diagnoses. Unfortunately, they couldn't be bothered to do the research and obliterated the diagnosis before such research could be performed.
Great comment never really looked up Schizoid Personality Disorder, the similarities with Asperger's is interesting.
I would bet many self diagnosed may have ScPD, or even many diagnosed hf level 1 or aspies really have this, but claim or are diagnosed autistic in error, however both are really paper diagnosis with no real biology separating Asperger's or even many idiopathic autism with ScPD.
https://neurodivergentinsights.com/misd ... -vs-autism
There's a schiZotypy Autism Questionnaire that's in development, so hopefully there will be a bit of clarity. But, separating the Aspies from the Schizoids is a bit of a moot point as the AS diagnosis was nuked in the DSM 5, whatever replaces the never to happen DSM 6 is almost certainly going to remove the ScPD diagnosis as it's not in active use with most of the people that had previously qualified for diagnosis now being moved to either Avoidant Personality Disorder or Paranoid Personality Disorders. With those that are closer to the Autism Spectrum being more or less completely left out.
It's rather unfortunate that there was so little care involved with respect to the issue because there has always been a legitimate question as to whether these are different groups, and a lot of the confusion between autism and schizophrenia is probably the result of these groups not being broken out into a sort of goldilocks sort of a diagnosis for those that aren't schizophrenic enough for the schizophrenia spectrum, but not really autistic enough for the autistic spectrum either. From the looks of it, that's not likely to be resolved any time soon and with both diagnoses likely to be gone in the near future, the effort that it's going to take to research is going to be substantially larger than it would have been had they opted to just leave things as they were pending further research.
I would not be surprised in the least if there weren't people that had been diagnosed AS back when that was a thing that had it show up a bit later, that might have been a better fit for ScPD, but weren't avoidant enough or paranoid enough to get recognized as being on that side of the boundary.
EDIT: For all practical purposes, the main difference between AS and ScPD appears to be age that the traits show up. Most of the rest of it seems to be pretty arbitrary. Neither is necessarily a disorder as such as a lot of these people can function, there's just significant social issues and depending on the specifics they can potentially get along just fine without 3rd party supports.
lostonearth35
Veteran
Joined: 5 Jan 2010
Age: 50
Gender: Female
Posts: 12,688
Location: Lost on Earth, waddya think?
ASPartOfMe
Veteran
Joined: 25 Aug 2013
Age: 67
Gender: Male
Posts: 35,980
Location: Long Island, New York
A New Autism Study May Not Say What You Think It Says
A new autism epidemiology paper in JAMA, “Autism Diagnosis Among U.S. Children and Adults, 2011-2022” has garnered significant coverage (USA Today headline: “Autism is spiking in young adults, study finds”) for its focus on adults with autism.
Behind the headlines, however, it’s tougher to glean just what this study is telling us. I argue that a closer look at the data shows scant evidence for a wave of new autism diagnoses in adults—or that there are vast numbers of autistic adults who have yet to be diagnosed.
Lead author Luke Grosvenor told USA Today>that “researchers involved in the study set out to assess how many adults had been diagnosed with autism because earlier studies largely focused on school-age children.” The paper itself says, “The greatest relative increase in diagnosis rate from 2011 to 2022 occurred among 26-to-34-year-olds (450%).”
What, exactly, does that mean? You might interpret it as saying that the increase among adults means the health systems identified and diagnosed a larger portion of existing autistic adults. If so, you’d be in good company. Andy Shih, the chief science officer of Autism Speaks, wrote on LinkedIn that this study is relevant to “particularly adults who may have gone undiagnosed in childhood.”
The study doesn’t say anything about previously undiagnosed adults, but that seems to be one possible interpretation. Another interpretation is that the number of autistic adults grew naturally as increasing numbers of autistic children grew older. Those two stories have very different implications in the real world.
The “recently diagnosed adults” story means there was little or no actual increase in autistic children over time. That is, there are vast numbers of undiagnosed adults and we need to identify them and provide them with appropriate services.
In contrast, according to the “increasing number of autistic children” story, the rate of occurrence of autism has increased dramatically over many years. That implies that something must have caused the increase, and it can’t be simply inherited. It also means that there are vastly fewer undiagnosed adults than some people have claimed.
Obviously, those two stories can’t both be correct. And which one is correct has huge implications for public policy and our priorities for autism research. How can you know which one is true?
The key to understanding autism epidemiology is to examine the data by birth year. Your year of birth is one of the few things about you that can never change. Your age, on the other hand, always increases by one every year.
When you compare people who were a certain age in different study years, you are comparing different people born in different years. Many papers show data organized by study year while few explicitly show birth year. You can easily convert one to the other: study year minus age equals birth year. For example, children who were eight years old in 2020 were born in 2012. This JAMApaper’s reported data are in terms of age, not birth year. But we can easily convert it to birth year.
To understand what the data has to say, we have to know what the numbers mean. Is it about rates that people were diagnosed? Or is it about prevalence—that is, how many autistic people there are at a given time? Those are different things. The paper uses both “diagnosis rates” and “prevalence.”
While the correct interpretation is not obvious, Grosvenor confirmed my guess. In fact, the data shows the proportions of members of the participating health networks who had an autism or ASD diagnostic code in their electronic health records in each study year. A diagnostic code does not mean that someone was diagnosed recently. It doesn’t necessarily mean they were ever diagnosed. Doctors treating patients decide what diagnostic codes to put in patients’ records. An autism or ASD diagnostic code might indicate that the patient was diagnosed at some point in the past, but that’s not entirely correct. We can treat it like prevalence.
To understand what the data has to say, we have to know what the numbers mean. Is it about rates that people were diagnosed? Or is it about prevalence—that is, how many autistic people there are at a given time? Those are different things. The paper uses both “diagnosis rates” and “prevalence.”
While the correct interpretation is not obvious, Grosvenor confirmed my guess. In fact, the data shows the proportions of members of the participating health networks who had an autism or ASD diagnostic code in their electronic health records in each study year. A diagnostic code does not mean that someone was diagnosed recently. It doesn’t necessarily mean they were ever diagnosed. Doctors treating patients decide what diagnostic codes to put in patients’ records. An autism or ASD diagnostic code might indicate that the patient was diagnosed at some point in the past, but that’s not entirely correct. We can treat it like prevalence.
The 26-34 group in the 2011 data was born in 1976 through 1984, while the 26-34 group in the 2022 data was born in 1987 through 1995. (In this paper’s data, the ages refer to the year before each study year.) Greater diagnosed prevalence in the later-born group vs. the earlier one could be due simply to greater prevalence among the later group.
But does the later-born group actually display a greater prevalence? We can answer that by looking more closely at the data.
The paper’s data has birth year prevalence information, but it takes a bit of effort to dig up. This JAMA paper’s eTable 7 gives the proportions of autistic patients (those with diagnostic codes) for each study year grouped into ranges of ages. I used an Excel spreadsheet to study the diagnostic code prevalence by individual birth years. The result is not as precise as we’d like, because of the age grouping, but it is still quite informative.
The following graph shows the birth year prevalence for birth years 1976 through 1995, encompassing both the earlier and later groups mentioned above. As I said, it’s not exactly prevalence but it’s the closest we can get with this data.
As you can see, there was a dramatic exponential increase in birth year prevalence: 12.3 percent per year. Note that the graph looks lumpy because the data is grouped into ranges of years.
The later-born group, born in 1987–1995, has an average birth year prevalence that is 3.82 times that of the earlier group born in 1976–1984. So, most, or possibly all, of the increase found between those two groups of adults likely came from the increasing birth year prevalence. That shows that the “recently diagnosed adults” interpretation is not correct.
Other data sources consistently show that as people from each birth year age, more are diagnosed, and this trend is similar for all birth years. So while the health systems may have diagnosed some members as adults—we can’t tell from this data—that is not something new. It has been going on for decades.
We can also examine the trend in birth year prevalence for all birth years.
This shows an increase of 7 percent per year of birth, which strikes me as fairly significant. It's almost the same increase as in the set of CDC autism reports on children born 8 years before each study year. The CDC reports show an increase of 7.4 percent per year from birth years 1992 through 2012.
Two scientific papers in the last few years purport to show there are vast numbers of undiagnosed adults. Both rely on the assumption that birth year prevalence has been constant all along.
I argue that both the data in this JAMA paper and the CDC reports contradict that assumption, meaning that there are relatively few undiagnosed adults now but there will be many more autistic adults in the future as the younger autistic people age. That says a lot about where public policy and research should focus.
_________________
Professionally Identified and joined WP August 26, 2013
DSM 5: Autism Spectrum Disorder, DSM IV: Aspergers Moderate Severity
“My autism is not a superpower. It also isn’t some kind of god-forsaken, endless fountain of suffering inflicted on my family. It’s just part of who I am as a person”. - Sara Luterman
Similar Topics | |
---|---|
Having Autism |
23 Nov 2024, 9:49 am |
PTSD or autism |
03 Nov 2024, 5:13 pm |
Autism and Fatigue? |
Today, 12:17 am |
Teenager with Autism and OCD |
21 Nov 2024, 8:52 am |