Page 1 of 1 [ 3 posts ] 

ASPartOfMe
Veteran
Veteran

User avatar

Joined: 25 Aug 2013
Age: 66
Gender: Male
Posts: 34,471
Location: Long Island, New York

25 Sep 2019, 10:20 am

Social camouflaging in autism: Is it time to lose the mask? by Will Mandy University College London for SAGE Journal

Quote:
Many autistic people feel obliged to pretend not to be autistic. They invest considerable effort daily in monitoring and modifying their behaviour to conform to conventions of non-autistic social behaviour. This phenomenon has come to be called ‘social camouflaging’, also referred to as ‘masking’, ‘compensation’ and ‘pretending to be normal’ (Hull et al., 2017; Lai et al., 2017; Livingston, Colvert, et al., 2019). Camouflaging strategies are diverse. Some are relatively simple, for example, when someone takes the decision not to do their favourite stims when among non-autistic people, or develops rules to use eye contact in a conventional manner. Other camouflaging strategies are elaborate and complex, developed via a careful, conscious and often protracted campaign of learning. An example would be a teenage autistic girl purposefully studying the behaviour of a non-autistic girl at school, and then, over time, adopting her attitudes, dress, gestures and facial expressions, thus developing a persona to navigate social situations.

Social camouflaging is widespread among autistic people. In one recent online study, 70% of autistic adult participants reported that they consistently camouflage (Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019). Despite how common camouflaging is, until recently autism researchers overlooked it. Of course, autistic people have long known about camouflaging. It features prominently in autistic life writing, notably in Liane Holliday-Willey’s (1999) best-selling memoir ‘Pretending to Be Normal’. Also, a small number of clinicians have, over the last few decades, shown an awareness of social camouflaging. Lorna Wing (1981), the eminent British psychiatrist and pioneer of autism practice, when considering the under diagnosis of autistic girls, puts forward the idea of camouflaging; several clinicians have subsequently written insightfully about this aspect of autistic experience, based on their work with autistic children and adults (e.g. Atwood, 2006; Gould, 2017). But researchers can be slow to catch on, and it is only in the last 5 years that we have seen the publication of rigorous studies that yield science-based insights into autistic camouflaging. In this editorial, I summarise some of the insights that have come from this small but burgeoning literature. The emerging findings offer important perspectives into what is, in my view, the most important question in autism research, namely, ‘how best can autistic people be helped to lead more satisfying, productive and enjoyable lives?’.

Reasons for camouflaging
Why do autistic people camouflage? When we asked this question of one autistic woman, she replied ‘I want to avoid the bullying mostly’ (Hull et al., 2017). This is a typical response across studies (e.g. Livingston, Shah, & Happé, 2019; Tierney, Burns, & Kilbey, 2016). For many autistic people, camouflaging is experienced as an obligation, rather than a choice. It is often motivated by a sense of alienation and threat, and frequently represents an attempt to avoid ostracism and attacks. Such findings offer a vivid reminder of the daily challenges autistic people face: they show us the pervasive difficulties of being autistic in a world that is shaped by the non-autistic majority. When an autistic person camouflages, in many cases it is an attempt to manage the mismatch between their natural way of being and the demands their social environment places on them. Everyone faces conflicts between authenticity and pragmatism, but for autistic people, this is often especially stark.

Consequences of camouflaging
Autistic people describe some benefits of camouflaging (e.g. Hull et al., 2017). It can enable its practitioners to achieve goals, such as getting an education and holding down a job. It can also help establish relationships, in particular with non-autistic people.

Despite the advantages of camouflaging, my reading of the literature is that, for most people, these benefits are outweighed by costs. Camouflaging can hide an individual’s difficulties, preventing them from being understood and helped. Unsurprisingly, camouflaging can delay or even prevent diagnosis – an individual who has become expert at pretending not to be autistic is less likely to be referred for an autism assessment and less likely to be diagnosed if they are assessed (Bargiela et al., 2016). This appears to play an important role in the diagnostic biases against autistic girls and women, who are more likely than boys and men to use camouflaging strategies (Hull et al., 2019).

Social camouflaging is associated with mental health challenges. In qualitative research, autistic people consistently link their camouflaging to experiences of anxiety and depression (e.g. Bargiela et al., 2016). Several quantitative studies show associations between camouflaging and internalising problems: autistic people who camouflage also tend to report higher rates of anxiety and depression (e.g. Cage & Troxell-Whitman, 2019; Livingston, Colvert, et al., 2019). One study found that self-reported camouflaging is associated with higher rates of suicidality (Cassidy, Bradley, Shaw, & Baron-Cohen, 2018).

It is not clear whether the association between camouflaging and mental health problems reflects an underlying causal relationship: does camouflaging drive the development of mental health problems? Studies addressing this question will involve testing putative mechanisms whereby camouflaging could lead to poor mental health. The qualitative literature already suggests some hypotheses. Camouflaging is commonly reported to be exhausting and stressful, potentially leading to autistic burnout (e.g. Livingston, Shah, et al., 2019; Tierney et al., 2016). Furthermore, the habitual practice of pretending not to be autistic can erode a person’s sense of identity (e.g. Hull et al., 2017) and can lead to their needs being misunderstood or overlooked entirely (Bargiela et al., 2016). In addition, Cage, Di Monaco, and Newell (2018) have suggested that camouflaging may mediate the relationship between social stressors (e.g. bullying and lack of autism acceptance) and subsequent anxiety and depression. Another possibility is that the association between camouflaging and mental health represents the effect of an unmeasured third variable, for example, being in an environment that is inhospitable to autistic people. It will be valuable to investigate empirically the mechanisms that potentially link camouflaging and mental health difficulties. Such work will not only yield insights into the effects of camouflaging, but can contribute answers to the wider question of why autistic people are at such high risk of suffering poor mental health.

Wider lessons from camouflaging research
I have argued that camouflaging represents one way in which autistic people manage the challenges they face while operating in social environments that are mainly shaped by non-autistic people. In one sense, camouflaging is adaptive. It is a creative, resourceful response to difficulties (Lai & Szatmari, 2019). But the evidence to date suggests that it is associated with exhaustion, stress, anxiety, depression, identity confusion and even suicidality. Thus, it would appear that the attempts that autistic people make to adapt to the non-autistic social world can be deleterious to well-being.

The study of camouflaging highlights a wider point about the role of the environment in the development of the difficulties that many autistic people face. Too often, the problems of autistic people have been assumed to arise simply from their own individual deficits. Camouflaging research reminds us that this is wrong: the problems autistic people face arise from a misfit between the individual’s unique pattern of strengths and difficulties, and the demands their environment places on them. For example, if an autistic boy refuses to go to school, this is a problem – but it is not a problem inherent to the child. Rather, it arises from an interaction between that child and the environment in which he operates. In this case, it may be that the school places social and cognitive demands upon the child that exceed his capacities, and does not make enough use of his strengths.

If we view the difficulties of autistic people using this more ecological, context-based approach, we can derive the principle that autism intervention should aim to improve the fit between the individual and their environment. Camouflaging research shows us that attempts to do this only by changing the individual, without modifying the environment, run the risk of being ineffective or, even worse, harmful. Unfortunately, to date, much of the evidence base for autism interventions focuses on interventions that place the onus on autistic individuals to change (Wong et al., 2015). These include a range of treatments that, in various ways, seek to make autistic people act or think differently. In developing the evidence base for supporting autistic people, we need to place greater emphasis on interventions that improve the person-environment fit by modifying the environment, rather than the person (e.g. Mandy et al., 2016).

Currently there is a lively social media conversation about camouflaging taking place among some autistic people, centred on the hashtag #TakeTheMaskOff. In this conversation, autistic people commonly state that they would like to be able to camouflage less. But it is also acknowledged that this can be difficult, as people fear the consequences of being more openly autistic, for example, at work or in social settings. An active campaign is needed to shape public attitudes and understanding, to increase autism acceptance and tolerance. This can help create more community, educational and work spaces where autistic people are freer to be themselves and have a better chance of actualising their talents. A measure of success in this venture will be if an increasing number of autistic people feel able to camouflage less.

The author thanks David Mandell, Meng-Chuan Lai, Laura Hull and Julia Cook, all of whom helped improve this editorial by giving feedback on an earlier draft.


_________________
Professionally Identified and joined WP August 26, 2013
DSM 5: Autism Spectrum Disorder, DSM IV: Aspergers Moderate Severity

It is Autism Acceptance Month

“My autism is not a superpower. It also isn’t some kind of god-forsaken, endless fountain of suffering inflicted on my family. It’s just part of who I am as a person”. - Sara Luterman


Mona Pereth
Veteran
Veteran

Joined: 11 Sep 2018
Gender: Female
Posts: 7,811
Location: New York City (Queens)

27 Sep 2019, 2:49 pm

Thanks for posting this. It's good the researchers are FINALLY noticing.

Hopefully it might even eventually occur to someone in the CHILDHOOD autism research establishment to start wondering if MAYBE teaching little autistic kids to imitate NT's MIGHT perhaps be something other than a completely unalloyed good???

We also, of course, need more autistic-friendly workplaces, so that more of us can survive without masking (beyond basic good manners and what I call autistic-friendly social skills, of course).


_________________
- Autistic in NYC - Resources and new ideas for the autistic adult community in the New York City metro area.
- Autistic peer-led groups (via text-based chat, currently) led or facilitated by members of the Autistic Peer Leadership Group.
- My Twitter / "X" (new as of 2021)


Ashariel
Veteran
Veteran

User avatar

Joined: 16 Jun 2012
Gender: Female
Posts: 1,779
Location: US

27 Sep 2019, 4:41 pm

Fantastic article - these points really spoke to me:

ASPartOfMe wrote:
Everyone faces conflicts between authenticity and pragmatism, but for autistic people, this is often especially stark.

Camouflaging can hide an individual’s difficulties, preventing them from being understood and helped.

Furthermore, the habitual practice of pretending not to be autistic can erode a person’s sense of identity

But the evidence to date suggests that it is associated with exhaustion, stress, anxiety, depression, identity confusion and even suicidality.


I deal with it by going out as little as possible.